The case against science is straightforward: much of the scientific literature, perhaps half, may simply be untrue. Afflicted by studies with small sample sizes, tiny effects, invalid exploratory analyses, and flagrant conflicts of interest, together with an obsession for pursuing fashionable trends of dubious importance, science has taken a turn towards darkness.
Richard Horton, editor of The Lancet

A study of language in Science articles from 1997 through 2021 raises concerns about exaggerated claims.

Careful scientists know to acknowledge uncertainty in the findings and conclusions of their papers. But in one leading journal, the frequency of hedging words such as “might” and “probably” has fallen by about 40% over the past 2 decades, a study finds.

If this trend holds across the scientific literature, it suggests a worrisome rise of unreliable, exaggerated claims, some observers say. Hedging and avoiding overconfidence “are vital to communicating what one’s data can actually say and what it merely implies,” says Melissa Wheeler, a social psychologist at the Swinburne University of Technology who was not involved in the study. “If academic writing becomes more about the rhetoric … it will become more difficult for readers to decipher what is groundbreaking and truly novel.”

The new analysis, one of the largest of its kind, examined more than 2600 research articles published from 1997 to 2021 in Science, which the team chose because it publishes articles from multiple disciplines. (Science’s news team is independent from the editorial side.) The team searched the papers for about 50 terms such as “could,” “appear to,” “approximately,” and “seem.” The frequency of these hedging words dropped from 115.8 instances per 10,000 words in 1997 to 67.42 per 10,000 words in 2021.

Less hedging may reflect a subtle strategy by authors to sell their results to editors and readers as an alternative to explicit exaggeration, according to the study, published in the August issue of Scientometrics. That should concern scientists, says co-author Ying Wei, a linguist at Nanjing University, because “essentially, the nature of academic knowledge is indeterminate.”

Science’s expectations that manuscripts provide reliable evidence didn’t drop during the study period, says Executive Editor Valda Vinson, who joined the journal in 1999. She suggests authors may be hedging less because editors have increasingly asked authors to supply additional supporting information. The Scientometrics findings may also reflect a move by the editors away from older conventions of scientific writing that encouraged hedging and passive voice, she says. That style has given way to a more modern, informal tone marked by less hedging. To avoid exaggeration, “we tone down language if it comes across as definitive when [the evidence] is not,” she adds.

Vinson also notes that the Scientometrics study looked only at papers published in the Research Article format, which used to be reserved for a few landmark papers that may have been especially likely to hedge. During the study period, Science increasingly shifted more findings to the Research Articles format.

A more informal writing style in research papers may have an upside if it makes them more understandable to policymakers and the public, Wheeler says. She and colleagues came to that conclusion in a 2021 Scientometrics study that analyzed the abstracts of nearly 800,000 articles published in psychology journals between 1970 and 2016. But the study also red flags a 24% increase during that period in an index for “clout.” This linguistic measure reflects language similar to “in my expert opinion,” that is confident and authoritative—perhaps in some cases too confident.

Although journal editors and reviewers should look out for exaggerated claims, they shouldn’t bear all the responsibility, Wheeler cautions. “It’s also up to universities and research institutes to value the quality over quantity of researchers’ outputs,” she says, “which would allow more time for academics to reflect and produce meaningful work instead of churning out as many publishable papers as possible.”

Wei adds a hedge of her own, however: The new study doesn’t show what caused the observed decline of hedging language. The pressure to publish that academics face to gain tenure, promotion, and professional recognition may play a role, but there could be other factors as well. The nature of the connection, she says, deserves further study.